effectiveness of a fluency intervention with english
play

Effectiveness of a Fluency Intervention with English Language - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Effectiveness of a Fluency Intervention with English Language Learners Elfrieda H. Hiebert University of California, Berkeley Aim of study to examine the efficacy of an intervention that had been effective with English speakers in schools


  1. Effectiveness of a Fluency Intervention with English Language Learners Elfrieda H. Hiebert University of California, Berkeley

  2. Aim of study to examine the efficacy of an intervention that had been effective with English speakers in schools where 90%* (or more) of students were classified as English Language Learners *California Department of Education, 2004

  3. Background: Theoretical  The underlying premise of the work is that, to be a fluent reader, automaticity is required with the words in texts that occur most in written English. Texts that “instantiate” a fluency curriculum can aid in the development of this automaticity.

  4. Background: Empirical Study 1: Hiebert & Fisher (May 2005): Texts from all studies in the National Reading Panel’s (NICHD, 2000) review of fluency instruction were identified and available exemplars analyzed. Analyses showed that texts with controlled vocabulary were used in 74% of the studies in the meta-analysis. Of the four studies that used literature, only one reported a fluency outcome and, in that study, treatment and comparison groups did not differ significantly. That is: the effect size for fluency came from studies that used texts with controlled vocabulary.

  5. Study 2 (Hiebert, 2006): Repeated reading treatments with literature (core reading program) and scaffolded texts were compared. The average gain of 2.5 words per week for the literature-text students exceeded expected growth rate by more than a word per week. The scaffolded-text students ’ gain of 3.1 words per week was 2.6 times that of expected growth. The scaffolded text treatment increased the percentage of students reaching 50th+ percentile by 15%, while the literature group increased by 1%.

  6. Study 3 ( Hiebert, 2005 ) : Repeated reading treatments with literature (core reading program) and scaffolded texts were compared as in Study 2 In addition, a passive control group read (but not repeatedly) from literature program. Both intervention groups made greater fluency gains than the control group; in addition, the scaffolded- text group made greater fluency gains than the literature group. The superior performance of the scaffolded-text treatment was accomplished in about 60% of the time spent on reading by the literature group.

  7. The Intervention  10 second-grade teachers (5 in two schools with 90+% native Spanish speakers) agreed to spend 15-30 minutes on the intervention daily for a 24-week period  The teachers received:  3 inservice sessions (prior to Week 1 and at Weeks 8 and 16) which emphasized reasons for intervention and procedure for three readings of a text (Read 1: Predict content; identify key vocabulary; read silently for big idea; Read 2: Summarize and choral read; Read 3: Timed read and identify what is to be remembered.  the same 3 sets of materials: • expository texts for the first 8 weeks; • narrative texts for the next 8 weeks; and • another set of the expository texts for the last 8 weeks.

  8. The Texts  Texts were analyzed according to the TExT model (types of words--high-frequency words within particular word zones and monosyllabic words with particular vowel patterns--and number of appearances of words. 500 most-frequent words + Single-appearing, multi- Words with “short” and “long” syllabic words (%) vowels (%) Set 1 89 0 (informational) Set 2 (narrative) 89 1 Set 3 87 0 (informational) Core reading 79 4 program

  9. Student Measures  TOWRE Sight Word and Phonemic Decoding: November and May  Fluency and Comprehension: November, February, and May with following for each point in time: •Narrative Words Correct Per Minute (WCPM) & Comprehension •Expository WCPM & Comprehension

  10. Fluency and Comprehension Measures  WCPM: Represents the average of two passages that came from a set of equivalent passages; similar to Fuchs et al. (1993), number of correct responses was used to establish WCPM  Comprehension: Correct responses as percentage of all responses

  11. Classroom Measures  Words Per Day (WPD) (Observed): An average of words read during intervention session (i.e., 15- 30 minutes) at 4 observation points  WPD (Teacher Report): An average based on teacher log [Both WPD indexes indicate number of words that students saw in daily intervention sessions]

  12. Research Questions Did students ’ WCPM & comprehension 1. show similar growth at three points in time? With narrative or expository texts? As a result of WPD? What percentage of students moved to 2. higher benchmark groups on WCPM? Did students ’ performances on norm- 3. referenced measures of sight word and phonemic decoding from beginning to end of intervention differ ?

  13. 1a: Did WCPM show similar growth at three points in time? For two types of texts? As a result of WPD? 70 Narrative 60 Expository 50 WCPM 40 30 20 10 0 Nov Feb May Raw WCPM & comprehension scores were analyzed using hierarchical level modeling (measurement at a time point, student, classroom, school). For WCPM: •students in each classroom started at same average level but advanced at different slopes (p < .05) but no significant effect for either of WPD measures •difference between kinds of kinds of texts was significant (p < .001 and for time F(1,8.94)=237.5, p<.0001; however, fluency growth for both types of texts advanced at same rate

  14. 1b: Did Comprehension show similar growth at three points in time? For two types of texts? As a result of WPD? 100 Narrative 80 Expository % Comprehension 60 40 20 0 Nov Feb May • The difference between kinds of texts (narrative and expository) was significant F(1,823)=59.1 p<.0001; The main effect for time was also significant F(1,823)=111.7, p<.0001. However: There was no significant interaction between time of measurement and text type. • There was no significant effect for any of the WPD measures. However, variance at the teacher level was significant (p<.05) (i.e., Students in different classes started at different levels). However the slopes were homogeneous; students advanced at the same slopes in classrooms.

  15. 2. What percentage of students moved to higher benchmark groups? Fluency Fluency Quartile Post Total Quartile Pre* 2 3 4 +: 37 (51-75) (26-50) (25 & students below) (22%) 2 6* 1 0 7 √ 109 (67.8%) 3 12 33 15 60 -16 (10%) 4 3 22 73 98 Total 21 56 88 165 *Includes 1 student from Quartile 1 (76-100)

  16. 2. What percentage of students moved to higher benchmark groups? Fluency Fluency Quartile Post Total Quartile 2 3 4 Pre* (51-75) (26-50) (25 & below) 2 6* 1 0 7 3 12 33 15 60 4 3 22 73 98 Total 21 56 88 165 *Includes 1 student from Quartile 1 (76-100)

  17. 3. Did students ’ performances on norm-referenced measures of sight word and phonemic decoding differ from beginning to end of intervention? Sight Words Phonemic Decoding Standard Raw Standard Raw Score* Score* Nov. 98 33.2 102 17.1 May 101 48.2 107 24.8 •p >.001 Raw scores were converted to standard scores to examine growth beyond expected growth associated with age. Paired sample t-tests showed significant differences. The same result was also found on a repeated measure ANOVA.

  18. Conclusions  WCPM and comprehension: difference between kinds of kinds of texts was significant as was growth over time  Benchmarks: Almost a quarter of students moved to a higher percentile group; 53% of the sample remained in the 4th quartile.  Norm-referenced measures of sight word recognition and phonemic decoding : students showed significant improvement as a result of the intervention; students are within the “average” range on these word-level measures

  19. Observations  The discrepancy between English Language Learners ’ word reading and text reading was substantial.  Policies (such as those of the CA English Language Arts Commission that has mandated decodable texts for middle graders who are struggling readers) appear not to be based on evidence of the literacy strengths and needs of native Spanish speaking English Language Learners.

Recommend


More recommend