conducting web based experiments for numerical cognition
play

Conducting web-based experiments for numerical cognition research - PowerPoint PPT Presentation

Conducting web-based experiments for numerical cognition research Arnold Kochari Institute for Logic, Language, and Computation, University of Amsterdam Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University in Nijmegen


  1. Conducting web-based experiments for numerical cognition research Arnold Kochari Institute for Logic, Language, and Computation, University of Amsterdam 
 Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behaviour, Radboud University in Nijmegen

  2. Web-based data collection for psychology What is possible: - surveys - reaction times - mouse tracking - audio/video recording Advantages: - fast: 
 - simultaneous data collection 
 - no need for appointment management - reaching diverse populations; large samples - cheaper (not because of underpaying) - easy sharing of the experiment scripts (no special software needed) Issues: 
 - experiment programming and participant recruitment 
 - no control of the environment in which experiment is completed - timing less accurate than in labs

  3. Timings of RT experiments in web-browsers - More variability in RTs due to imprecise timings (different monitors, keyboards, browsers) - Clear lag in RTs measured by JavaScript (2-45 ms - Reimers & Stewart 2015; 25 ms - de Leeuw & Motz 2016) 
 However: 
 - it is random 
 - this lag is stable across conditions / within-systems 
 - we can compensate for that by having more participants not a big issue for within-participant designs between-participant effects can still be reliable, just with more participants Replications of classical effects in web-based experiments: - Crump et al 2013 (stroop task, task switching, flanker task, simon task, attentional blink, masked priming etc.) 
 - Zwaan and Pecher 2012 (mental simulation in language comprehension) 
 - Barnhoorn et al 2014 (stroop, masked priming, attentional blink)

  4. Technical questions

  5. Programming experiments for web-browsers Free and open source: • jsPsych - scripting manually • lab.js - graphical interface + scripting manually • PsychoPy/PsychoJS - graphical interface + scripting manually • PsyToolkit - also takes care of data collection Commercial: • Gorilla - graphical interface Importantly: same scripts can be used on lab computers

  6. Data collection tools Hosting data collection: • combined with the experiment programming platform (e.g. Gorilla.sc) • special dedicated service (e.g. JATOS, psiTurk) • personal or university web hosting space Participant recruitment: • Amazon MTurk (not available from every country) • Prolific.ac • Qualtrics • others

  7. Results with 2 classical paradigms

  8. 
 
 
 Size-congruity effect close replication of Henik & Tzelgov 1982 Materials: 8 digit pairs 
 Experimental factors: 
 congruity (congruent vs. incongruent) 
 X numerical distance (2 vs 4) 
 X font size distance (small vs. large) 
 - 64 unique exp. trials + neutral and empty trials 
 - trial: fixation cross for 150 ms followed by digit pairs for max. 1850 ms 
 - participants used P and Q as response keys Experiment 1a: number comparison task ; N participants = 23 
 Experiment 1b: size comparison task ; N participants = 24 
 - average time spent on task: 6-8 minutes

  9. Size-congruity effect: numerical distance Current study: Henik & Tzelgov, Exp 2: Semantic comparison * main e ff ect of congruity 
 * main e ff ect of numerical distance Physical comparison * main e ff ect of congruity 
 * interaction of congruity and physical 
 size distance Experiment scripts, data and analysis code are available at: http://osf.io/dy8kf

  10. Size-congruity effect: physical size distance Henik & Tzelgov, Exp 2: Current study: Physical comparison + * main e ff ect of physical size distance 
 + * interaction of congruity and physical 
 relevant dimension size distance Semantic comparison to be ignored dimension + * interaction of congruity and physical 
 size distance Experiment scripts, data and analysis code are available at: http://osf.io/dy8kf

  11. 
 Distance and priming effects replication of Van Opstal, Gevers, de Moor, & Verguts 2008 500 ms 100 ms 83 ms 100 ms max. 2000 ms Materials: all digits from 1 to 9 except 5 included as primes and targets; 
 64 unique combinations Experimental factors: 
 distance of the target from the standard (1-4) 
 distance of the prime from the standard (1-4) 
 congruity 
 - 256 trials in total 
 - participants N = 72 - participants used P and Q as response keys 
 2 di ff erent possible mappings 
 - average time spent on task: 15 minutes

  12. Distance and priming effects: 
 distance of the target Van Opstal et al (exp 1): Current study: - only trials with identical prime and target analysed - 2 (size: before/after the standard) X 4 ( abs. comparison distance: 1, 2, 3, or 4) within- subjects ANOVA on median correct RTs 
 - main e ff ect of comparison distance: F (3, 213) = 10.6, p < 0.001. Experiment scripts, data and analysis code are available at: http://osf.io/dy8kf

  13. Distance and priming effects: 
 congruity before standard: Van Opstal et al (Exp 1): 405 congruent: =22 ms (median) 427 incongruent: after standard: congruent: 399 =30 ms incongruent: 429 Current study: before standard: 528 congruent: =24 ms 552 incongruent: after standard: congruent: 538 =22 ms incongruent: 560 - only trials with non-identical prime and target analysed - 2 (size: before/after the standard) X 2 (congruency) within-subjects ANOVA on median correct RTs 
 - main e ff ect of congruency: F (1, 71) = 58.4, p < 0.001. Experiment scripts, data and analysis code are available at: http://osf.io/dy8kf

  14. Distance and priming effects: 
 distance of the prime Current study: Van Opstal et al (exp 1): - only congruent trials with non-identical prime and target analysed - 2 (size: before/after the standard) X 4 (abs. priming distance: 1, 2, 3, or 4) within-subjects ANOVA on median correct RTs 
 - main e ff ect of prime distance: F (3, 213) = 13.9, p < 0.001. NB: Primes were not actually displayed for exactly 83 ms! Experiment scripts, data and analysis code are available at: http://osf.io/dy8kf

  15. Lessons learnt

  16. Some tips - Fair pay is important - Not too many trials. I try to have experiments for max 15-20 minutes. - Pre-register participant exclusion criteria: a lot of researcher degrees of freedom here. - Ensuring participants put e ff ort: 
 - I automatically exclude everyone who spent less than X s on reading the instructions 
 - 50% error rate 
 - a question at the end where they can given an honest answer - Ensuring participants do not get distracted: - tell them their data will be lost if they switch windows/tabs 
 - experiment has an automatic pace - no opportunity to decide to do something else 
 - inspect the duration of breaks (>3 minutes means they got distracted) - Ensuring participant naiveté: 
 - put a cap on the number of previous studies

  17. Experiment scripts, data and analysis code are available at: 
 http://osf.io/dy8kf

  18. References • Barnhoorn, J. S., Haasnoot, E., Bocanegra, B. R., & van Steenbergen, H. (2015). QRTEngine: An easy solution for running online reaction time experiments using Qualtrics. Behavior research methods, 47(4), 918-929. • Crump, M. J., McDonnell, J. V., & Gureckis, T. M. (2013). Evaluating Amazon's Mechanical Turk as a tool for experimental behavioral research. PloS one, 8(3), e57410. • de Leeuw, J. R., & Motz, B. A. (2016). Psychophysics in a Web browser? Comparing response times collected with JavaScript and Psychophysics Toolbox in a visual search task. Behavior Research Methods, 48(1), 1-12. • Henik, A., & Tzelgov, J. (1982). Is three greater than five: The relation between physical and semantic size in comparison tasks. Memory & cognition, 10(4), 389-395. • Reimers, S., & Stewart, N. (2015). Presentation and response timing accuracy in Adobe Flash and HTML5/JavaScript Web experiments. Behavior research methods, 47(2), 309-327. • Van Opstal, F ., Gevers, W., De Moor, W., & Verguts, T. (2008). Dissecting the symbolic distance e ff ect: Comparison and priming e ff ects in numerical and nonnumerical orders. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15(2), 419-425. • Zwaan, R. A., & Pecher, D. (2012). Revisiting mental simulation in language comprehension: Six replication attempts. PloS one, 7(12), e51382.

Recommend


More recommend